• Good quality data for making good diagnoses

    Resumen


    • Patients do not have any obligation to be the good patients that practitioners want.

    • We must know our tolerance to 'uncertainty' (critical strain), and the pressure that patients sometimes put us under ('to heal me!'), to give ourselves more time and not to prematurely finish the consultation.


Autor/es: Francesc Borrell i Carrió. Traducido por Dra. Elena Barquero Bardon y Dra. Heather Beaumont
Fecha: 15-04-2016
ISBN: No disponible

Índice

Good quality data for making good diagnoses

Key Ideas

 

  • Patients do not have any obligation to be the good patients that practitioners want.

  • We give more value to strategies to simplify and shorten the interview time, than strategies which minimise the risk of clinical error.

  • The history taking should not end until the clinician is able to write a report specifying, at least, the chronology and nature of the problem.

  • History taking by pathographic viewing: chronology (film of events), plot (soundtrack, what happens), impact (relieving factors).

  • We need to automate groups of questions, depending on the clinical situation, so it hardly requires us any effort to put them into practice, in particular what we call the 'psychosocial jump'.

  • The physical examination is part of the interpersonal relationship and it begins when a patient enters the consulting room. By listening and observing, we are already examining the patient.

  • We must know our tolerance to 'uncertainty' (critical strain), and the pressure that patients sometimes put us under ('to heal me!'), to give ourselves more time and not to prematurely finish the consultation.

  • Pathology of frequent attendance: 'you come so often, I often disregard you'.

  • Starting from scratch with a frequently attending patient is a true exercise of humility!

  • Being careful in formulating a diagnosis gives us the freedom to modify it more easily in the future.

  • What is the main bioethical challenge of a clinician? getting up from the chair to assess patients over and over again!

 

 

Basic Skills to get quality data

 

Watching is to set our sight on something, listening is to attend, but understanding is not only being attentive, but it has some recreation. The clinician who can understand endorses the patient's materials, plays them in their own imagination and recreates them, as if they were partly their own experiences. We found that a good clinician is someone who before understanding others has understood, has explored and has heard himself. Anyone who claims to know something about the world by only projecting what they have heard or read is delusional! We can only glimpse the world, the patient, and others in general, through our experiences. Our body and our own emotions are the filter, and the inescapable musical instrument to internalise and understand others. We have an image of the patient which we compare with similar images of the patient and we internalise this and interpret it with our own emotions: 'how would I feel if...?' We have a deeper understanding of the patient at this point and this process enables us to understand and share the patient’s experiences and emotions i.e. to empathise. One of the keys to intuitive thinking may lie within this; internalising by analogy. But that is not all; the clinician does not renounce the semiological data which is an objective analysis, is purely external and is based on well-established criteria. In order to listen in a semiological manner we must acquire very complex practices. We will devote this chapter to this. Give us good data, allow ourselves to go beyond our first impressions and allow our intelligence to work. We will defend a concept of expertise based on a continued contrast between intuitive thought and thought based on criteria.

 

All that will be taken care of in this chapter, in which we take the semi-structured interview for the exploratory part as a starting point. Remember that the tasks are:

 

  1. Review the patient's past medical history or the patient’s previous consultations in their medical records.

  2. Warm greeting.

  3. Establishing the presenting complaint. Map of demands and complaints. Pathobiography

  4. Active listening ('the interview leak point')

  5. Find and complete data.

  6. Summary of the information obtained.

  7. Physical examination, if any.

 

In Chapter 2 we reached the fourth point, and in this chapter we will address items 5, 6 and 7, with an emphasis on semiologic skills, through the sphere of the rational-emotive model of the clinical act

 

Identifying and completing the data. When questioning is appropriate: Skills during the history taking

Any interviewer should effortlessly use four packages of questions (Table 3.1).

 

Table 3.1.

The four packages of questions for history taking

First package: active listening skills

Technical types

 When it becomes necessary

Showing interest

When we are interested in viewing the world and the patient's experiences without influencing the narration of the facts

Facilitators

Very useful at the beginning of the interview

Phrases by repetition

When the presenting complaint is unclear

Empathy

When it appears that the patient wants to entrust us with 'sensitive material'

Suggested addition technique

When the patient finds it difficult to talk

Second package: questions for taking a focussed history

Question type

 When it becomes necessary

How is your pain

 

When did the pain start and chronology of their pain

When we want to establish facts regarding their symptoms

Where does it radiate  

The presenting complaint is very clear

What makes the pain better or worse  

There is a symptom or sign guide

Associated symptoms

 

Risk factors

 

Third package: questions for taking a psychosocial history

Question type

When it becomes necessary

What is your sleep like?

When we want to establish the psychological impact of the patient's discomfort

How is your mood?

When we suspect psychosocial stress or psychological difficulties

Do you have any concerns or worries?

There are many mottled symptoms in the patient's narrative

The patient who consults very frequently

Do you have any serious problems at work or at home?

The patient gives clues that guide us to this area

Fourth package: questions for a patient-centred interview

Question type

When it becomes necessary

What are your ideas about your symptoms?

 We want to know the impact of the illness on the patient.

How are things affecting your life?

 When the patient expresses concern, gloominess or anger.

How Do you think we can help you? What should we do to improve the situation? something which they don't quite express

 When we have the impression that the patient wants

Has something happened that Has changed your life? Or Has something happened that Has had a big impact on you?

 

Any are there any problems at home Or at work

 We suspect problems in the home which are affecting you? or work environment

 

These packages are to be learned in such way that they then arise almost naturally. How do we know whether we have grasped these techniques properly? Here are some indicators:

 

Active listening package

An indicator that this package of skills has borne its fruit is that the patient starts to talk in a more open way, providing longer answers. We have already spoken of this package in Chapter 2, so we will not expand here.

 

Package for focused histories

An indicator that this package of skills has borne fruit is that we have enough data to be able to write a clinical report with the features and chronology of the patient's symptoms. The patient considers the history taking to be the specific part of the healing art and that the Doctor will diagnose them with just the history taking. However, sometimes the clinician does this it in an incomplete way, as we will see later.

 

Package for the psychosocial history

A mistake is to think that this package should be activated only when the patient presents with psychological problems or problems with the social or family environment. In fact, every patient with chronic pain, patients who attend very often or patients with vague symptoms, can benefit from a psychosocial assessment. An indicator that this package bears fruit is that we can outline elements of stress or emotional aspects of the patient. It is important to distinguish a real resistance to talk about psychosocial issues ('I’d prefer not to talk about this topic'), fear that we relate the patient's symptoms to this area ('everybody says that this is anxiety and I don’t think this is anxiety at all'). In the latter case, we can try to prevent this resistance to talking about psychosocial issues by starting with a focussed history and, before making the jump to the psychosocial aspects, with sign posting by saying something like: 'well, now I would like to find out a bit more about you as a person... 'Do you have any hobbies? Which ones?... and do you sleep well?... how is your mood?», etc. Another strategy to start talking about psychosocial issues is to find out the quality of their sleep. On the other hand, it would be a mistake to ask: 'and what about your anxiety?’

 

A patient-centred package

It is a package which is not usually activated at first, as it could lead to some confusion. Sometimes we ask the patient: 'what do you think is happening to you?' And the patient answers: 'I don't know; that is why I have come to see you'. We name this response as the boomerang effect, and it should be followed with: ‘yes, of course, but I would like to know your opinion, what you think about it or what you might have discussed with your friends or family about this' (returning the boomerang). One indicator that this package is properly applied is that the flow of communication with the patient improves.

 

The big challenge for this part of the interview is to collect quality data. It is important that this data is real data, effectively sensed or experienced by the patient; reliable, meaning, that if another interviewer asks they will get similar responses, and valid, meaning, the data collected should be the data we are trying to obtain. In other words:

 

  • Real data: this patient... tells me things that have indeed happened?

  • Reliable data: another interviewer ... will they get similar answers?

  • Valid data: Is the data I am trying to obtain to get a diagnosis, useful for this patient and this clinical situation?

 

Basic techniques to get real, reliable and valid data

The most important thing is to create an atmosphere focused on the patient, avoiding 'yes' or 'no' questions almost automatically. In general, the more detail provided, the more realistic and reliable the data.

 

A concentration of closed questions diminishes the appearance of care focused on the patient. For example:

 

I: Does it happen more often in the morning?

P: Yes.

I: And when it happens, do you feel sick?

P: Yes.

I: Have you ever seen blood in your vomit?

P: No, no...

 

Here we see closed questions which can be answered in monosyllables. The risk of the patient responding 'to please' the interviewer is very high. If one of the answers has gone against the interviewer’s ‘expectation’, for example: 'now that you mention it, I once saw some blood, yes...’ this will undoubtedly have great diagnostic value.

 

Conversely, open questions require some verbalisation. Unlike closed questions, they cannot be answered monosyllabically. Some examples would be: 'how is the pain?’, ‘what were you doing when the pain started?' etc. We must avoid mistakes by adding a postscript which 'closes' the question; for example 'how is the pain? Is it stabbing?', so the question becomes a closed question. Do not forget the juxtaposition of techniques principle, discussed above, whereby when two techniques are juxtaposed, the one used last is always the predominant one, in this case, the closed question.

 

It is also a mistake to rush to start asking closed questions without first offering a ‘menu’ of suggested responses. A menu of suggested responses means offering 'clues' indicating the type of response expected. For example: 'Is the pain sharp, like a bite or as if someone is squeezing you?', ' did it happen years, days or months ago?' etc.

 

The menu of suggestions doesn't have to advise the patient about what we view as the most logical or plausible response. If, for example, we say: 'Has this been happening for months or for days?' we are implying that we expect the problem to have been happening for a short period of time and we would not accept: 'no, it is been going on for many years' as a consistent answer. For this reason, we prefer an illogical sequence of suggested answers such as: 'years, days or months?'

 

Another common mistake is to formulate a menu of suggestions that remains as only a single suggestion, either because we cannot think of more, or because when going to mention the second suggestion, the patient interrupts and says: 'yes, it is exactly as you’ve just said'. In both cases, we will reformulate the menu, in order to verify that, the patient indeed meant what they said.

 

Closed questions are those which we use most often. The value of the information they provide is very variable, depending on the degree of suggestibility of the patient and whether we use other techniques to check the validity of the information'. But sometimes we can't avoid them, especially when we are interested in a very specific aspect of the history. Its formulation is obvious: 'Does it happen in the morning?', 'Does it hurt here?' etc.

 

Such questions have to be complemented with open questions and the menu of suggestions. It is useful to follow a positive answer from the patient to a closed question with an open question. For example, if we ask: 'do you have a burning sensation when passing urine?' and the patient answers: 'yes, I do’, it would be appropriate to confirm this information by asking for some more information 'could you tell me more about that?'. If you tend to ask closed questions, you can easily transform them into 'a menu of suggestions’ adding '...or perhaps the opposite?’ For example, 'does it hurt before eating...or after eating? Is the pain in your stomach...or in other parts of your tummy?’ The resulting sentences won't be included in the annals of the Royal College, but the final effect is appropriate for the purpose that we pursue: avoiding mechanical 'yes and no answers'.

 

Even more dangerous are the induced response questions. For example, the professional says:

 

  • I assume you have not had chest pain here, right?

  • Yeah, but basically you seem cheerful, don't you?

  • I assume you have never vomited blood, have you?

 

Instead, open questions with specific content have clear advantages. Observe, in the first example, an interviewer with no special skills, and the same patient in hands of a skilled interviewer.

 

Interview with a novice

P: You insist this is depression, but I have continued pains and they don't disappear, I am suffering. And I have so much to do at home!

I: Are you tearful?

P: Of course I am. This is due to how I see myself.

I: Does your husband help you?

P: My husband has a lot of work. He gets up at 6 am and he doesn't come back until dinner time.

I: But, do you have a good relationship with your husband? Do you communicate well?

P: He is bored with my complaints, as you can imagine.

 

Interview with an expert

/1/ P: You insist this is depression, but I have continued pains and they don't disappear, I am suffering. And I have so much to do at home!

/2/ I (showing some empathy): I see... (Accompanying the transition): We will come back to your pain. Now, I would like to discuss another topic, for me to get to know you better, (open question): what do you do when you are alone?

/3/ P: I like listening to music. I used to dance when I was home alone (laughing). I used to go out with some friends for a walk and we used to have a coffee in the park.

/4/ I (showing some empathy): That is fine. (Open question): And when your husband comes back home, what does he usually do?

/5/ P: He has a shower and we have dinner.

/6/ I (closed questions): Do you talk while having dinner?

/7/ P: We watch TV. It is the only time of day we can watch sport.

/8/ I (confirmatory sentence): Of course, do you also enjoy watching football?

/9/ P: No, I don't like it at all, but what can I do?

/10/ I (open question with behavioural signalling value): And at night, what type of person is your husband?

/11/ P: He goes about his business, he does as he pleases, you understand ...

/12/ I (closed question): Do you mean if he wants to make use of your marital relationship?

/13/ P: Yes...

/14/ I (confirmatory sentence ): But you do not want to do so.

/15/ P (looking down): No ...

/16/ I (closed question with emotional signalling value): Have you ever been afraid of him?

 

The patient begins to cry, and from this point the clinician can establish a picture of domestic abuse.

 

Note the use of closed and inconclusive questions from the first interviewer versus a more open technique from the second interviewer but, at the same time the second interview contains a very specific content. In particular we can point out from the second interview the following interventions:

 

/2/I (showing some empathy): I see... (Accompanying the transition): We will come back to your pain. Now, I would like to discuss another topic, for me to get to know you better, (open question): what do you do when you are alone?

 

With this intervention the interviewer cleaves the patient’s reality into two levels: the level of complaints, which he promises to come back to later, and the level of everyday life, which shifts the focus of attention. Thus, he manages to lower the defences of the patient and gets into the social aspects by talking about her hobbies. The following questions or interventions are also very specific, getting the interviewer literally into the patient's reality. He wants to see what happens and the feelings that the patient has in these circumstances. We recommend to the reader that it is this effort to see what is happening, more than a particular technique, which guides the interviewer. All this allows him to say:

 

/10/ I: And at night, what type of person is your husband?

This is an open question, but a very specific one and with a well defined symbolic content. Thus, we should add that it has a behavioural signalling value and possibly emotional signalling because ultimately, it will make the patient think about her emotional and sexual relationships. It would have also been appropriate to ask questions like: Is he affectionate towards you, for example does he hug you? Does he say loving things? Etc. Finally, to get into the topic of domestic abuse:

 

/16/ I: Have you ever been afraid of him?

 

This question is a true 'trump', of high performance. In table 3.2 we summarise other 'aces' indicating the clinical settings in which they can be used.

 

Other ways to get enquire about marital relations: 'Is it a pleasant relationship? Have you ever felt that his comments hurt you?'. Note that in all situations in Table 3.2, it is essential to draw a picture with concrete data of the patient's reality (The pathographic display technique, which we will talk about later)

 

Table 3.2

Trumps


Intervention

Have you ever thought about harming yourself?

Has a family member (or your partner) ever complained about your alcohol intake?  

It is easy to miss taking your tablets, has this happened to you?

What type of person is your husband at night?

 Have you ever felt scared of him?

Clinical context

A suspicion of suicidal ideation

Alcohol misuse suspected

We suspect poor adherence to medication

Sexual problems suspected

A suspicion of domestic abuse


 

 

 

The importance of taking a focussed history. Chronology and associated symptoms.

Work on evaluation of clinical competence highlighted a deficit in taking a focussed history and doing a physical examination (Sunol R, 1992, Barragan N, 2000, Borrell C, 1990; Prados JA, 2003). Even experienced clinicians take for granted too much data. They don't find out the chronology of events in sufficient detail, and don't look for associated symptoms. This in part is due to a lack of time. We give more value to simplifying interview strategies than strategies that minimise the risk of a clinical error.

 

 

We have mentioned that focused history taking does not end until the clinician is able to write a report detailing the onset of the symptoms, the nature of the symptoms and the variation of the symptoms over time, trigger factors which worsen or improve the symptom, and any associated symptoms.

 

Any objective data (signs) or subjective data (symptoms) can lead to the diagnosis, we mean; it can become a guide symptom or sign. Let's see some technical aspects necessary to obtain quality data with the method we call pathographic display:

 

  1. Chronology (film of events). The inexperienced clinician usually finds out in detail the quality of the symptom(s). But they often forget the chronology or the timeline of events: when it started, whether it was intermittent and if there were intervals with no symptoms, if it has happened before, if it is improving or worsening. However finding the film of the facts is invaluable. A headache that lasts three months and gradually becomes worse to the point of disturbing sleep should alert us. Be precise: a date is better than 'weeks'. Be thorough: Is always the same intensity? Do you have asymptomatic intervals? Etc.
  2. Plot (soundtrack). Knowing the film of events we are going to add the soundtrack. What is your pain like? How strong is it? Where is it? Does it radiate anywhere? As we said previously, we will move from open questions to the menu of suggestions and finally to closed questions (Figure 3.1). For example, when we ask about a headache it is preferential to ask: ‘please can you point to the area of your head which hurts most' to 'does it hurt here? (Pointing to a specific part). If, for example, we say: 'does the entire head hurt (emphasising it) or just here?’ the patient could understand, at a non-verbal level, that we expect to be 'all' of their head. It would be, therefore, a question with an induced response. We will invite the patient to locate the discomfort in their body, preferring to point an area than a verbal reference. Occasionally, we will ask the patient to describe whether the discomfort is superficial or deep, and if 'it goes anywhere else', a sentence that is better understood that the word 'radiation'. We will always try to avoid medical jargon such as 'dyspepsia', 'gastritis', ‘migraine’, even if we think the patient will understand it.
  3. Biographic impact (relieving factors). And now we need to find out the relieving factors. This is of vital importance in relation to the symptoms, factors that exacerbate or relieve the symptoms, what the patient does to improve their symptoms. Don't forget to enquire about the psychosocial aspects in this part and also the hypothesis that the patient has constructed to explain what is happening. Whenever we can we will quantify the patient's suffering (for example, distance the patient is able to walk without discomfort, the number of steps the patient can take without having to stop, etc.). The importance of the symptoms to the patient or the impact on the patient will be established from things that the patient stops doing or does in a limited way: 'what impact does the have pain on your life?’ We will find out which factors may exacerbate or relieve the pain: 'have you noticed anything that makes your pain better? Or worse?’ Finally, ask about the presence or absence of associated symptoms: for example, in the case of epigastric pain: 'what is the pain like after eating?’ Associated symptoms are closely related to each presenting complaint and their differential diagnoses. In general, the positivity or negativity of a symptom associated opens the door of a diagnosis to us (for example, abdominal discomfort which improves in the absence of dairy products may indicate lactose intolerance, or the presence of blood in chronic diarrhoea points to inflammatory bowel disease), hence the importance of knowing the cluster of questions and the exploratory data literally hanging from every symptom or presenting complaint. A good guide can be found at Kraytmann (1983). In any case, we must give ourselves time to open our library of clinical situations and retrieve this information. A good technique for this is the Summary of information obtained.

 

Fig-3.1

Figure 3.1

Use of techniques to obtain specific data

 

 

Summary of information obtained.

This technique increases the reliability, reality and validity of the data obtained. We offer the patient a summary of the data, adding a question at the end: ‘Do you think that this summary is a good reflection of what is happening? Have we forgotten to add anything? Would you remove anything?

 

The use of this technique has surprising results. The patient feels listened to, but also participates directly in the final picture which forms in our heads. The flow of communication and the quality of data are also reinforced. Note the following example of a patient who believed he had a brain tumour and complained of constant headaches:

 

I: I'm going to make a summary of the information that I've obtained from your symptoms. Please listen to me carefully and if I say something that is not right, don’t hesitate to correct me. If I have understood things correctly, you have had a headache for three months, located in the front of your head and which is worse in the afternoons. You’ve almost never had discomfort in the morning, although you are having the pain almost daily, is that right? But it seems for three weeks in October, you were almost well and didn’t need to take any painkillers, did you? The three weeks when your headaches were better, coincided with a trip to the countryside. On the other hand, when you have a headache and you take paracetamol this gives little relief, even taking naproxen doesn’t help the pain... am I going in the right direction?

 

In this case, the interviewer did not include in the summary that these headaches had started after the patient heard that a friend had been diagnosed with a brain tumour, as he didn’t want to prematurely imply that the diagnosis had a psychological aspect. He decided to talk about it in another moment of the relationship. On the other hand, he took good care to confirm that for three weeks the patient had been pain free, as this data along with the absence of morning headaches, makes a diagnosis of intracranial hypertension much less likely (negative predictive value).

 

 

Physical examination, if appropriate

Physical examination begins when the patient comes into the room. Listening to the patient is already exploring. Observing also is exploring. Far from dividing the exploratory part of the interview into history taking and physical examination, we assume that they are two facets of the same argument. In addition, physical examination is part of the interpersonal relationship. What can we say about a pair of hands that do not know how to approach a painful abdomen? This undoubtedly discredits all the academic qualifications which may hang on the wall of the consulting room. Physical examination has also a deep symbolic meaning. It's about getting to know another reality, the patient’s reality and to some extent, all about their privacy. There's a common patient reaction when the clinician doesn't perform a physical examination. They usually say something like: «He didn’t even look at me». There's the possibility that the clinician has formed an adequate idea of the problem after half an hour of thorough focused history taking, but from the patient's point of view the clinician hasn't even bothered to “look at me” since there hasn't been any physical contact. The opposite happens when the patient comments: “he has examined me properly”. Physical examination in this context always means exploration. A psychiatrist, for example, is never going to examine in the same manner, not even when their psychopathological exploration has been spotless. Questions can often overwhelm the patient, but examination pacifies them. “A specialist did the same thing as you’ve just done”, could be the kind of comment a happy patient makes to their GP. One of the great things of the clinical act above all is this recognition of physicality, identifying with the other person (and to some extent, putting ourselves in other person's shoes).

 

We understand by basic physical examination the group of techniques that has a maximal yield for detecting prevalent diseases in each age group and sex. This group of techniques makes us used to the patient's body, its characteristics and what the patient's understands as normal or abnormal. On the other hand, a problem based physical examination (PBPE) (Borrell F, 2002, a, b) consists of a selection of different manoeuvres, orientated around the symptoms whose purpose is to guide us to the cause of the symptoms. For example: ' This patient has pins and needles in both hands, so I must rule out the possibility of carpal tunnel syndrome with Phalen’s and Tinel’s tests.

 

The best performance of a physical examination occurs when the basic physical examination is combined with the problem based physical examination. On the other hand, it is a mistake to think that history taking goes first and then the physical examination. In reality, as we examine the patient some hypothesis may occur to us and to corroborate them, we combine questions with examination manoeuvres. This is what we call history taking integrated in the physical examination.

 

Practical example: a 'broken' patient

Abbreviations: D: doctor; P: patient

TASK

DIALOGUE

Reading the most important data and planning the objectives

Doctor reviews the patient's medical records. He sets his own goals: 'it's been long time since this patient last attended and he hasn’t had the minimum health promotion done, this must be done'.

Cordial greeting

 D (shaking hands): How are you Mr Saunders?

 The patient defines the presenting complaint

 P: Fine thanks, I come as I am concerned about something... I'm afraid I have broken something, I am 'broken'.

Preventing additional demands

D: I will examine you shortly but, is there anything else you would to ask me about?

P: No, no

D: Tell me more about this pain.

Active listening, vanishing point

P: I was carrying a heavy weight and I felt a severe pain in my groin.

D: When did this happen?

P: A month ago

Makes a focused history of the groin discomfort, but when some discordant data appears, he also integrates it into the focused history.

 

D: Since then, how have you been feeling?

P: Fine, but I am constipated.

D: Do you feel a lump or do you have pain in the groin?

P: No I don’t have a lump or pain, but I feel very constipated and someone told me that this is a symptom of an obstructed hernia.

  Ascertains and completes data

 

D: Did you open your bowels every day before?

P: Yes, I think so...

D: Do you remember having any previous episodes of constipation like this one?

P: No, I don't.

D: How long have you been constipated?

P: Around three weeks...

 

Summary

D: Let's see... if I've understood this correctly, you think you are 'broken' because you felt a pain when you lifted a heavy weight, but you don't have any discomfort in the groin. You are concerned about your constipation.

P: Yes, this is.

Ascertains and completes data

D: Have you lost any weight?

P: No, no. But during the night, I have to get up and go to the fridge. I eat anything I have in front of me.

Tries to jump to the psychosocial aspect

D: Do you feel hungry or do you feel anxious?

P: I feel anxious, worried, I don't know... well... my daughter has left home, things happen.

Suggested addition with signalling value

D: But you cannot stop worrying.

The patient returns to their physical symptoms and the clinician respects this 'resistance' to talk about the psychological aspects

Yes, I think this makes me more constipated. I open my bowels once or twice a week now, but I used to open my bowels every day.

Activates focused history, concentrating on the constipation again

D: Do you have hard stools?

P: Yes, sometimes when I open my bowels I can see blood in my stool...but I have haemorrhoids.

D: What is the blood like?

P: The colour?...It is red, bright red and around the stools. This has been happening this week, but it has never happened before.

D: have you tried a laxative or anything like that?

P: No, my wife says I could try an onion enema, what do you think?

Attempt a psychosocial jump again, being very careful not to force the patient’s resistance.

D: Well, we can talk about this later. Now I would like to know how your mood is.

P: I am always very positive, but after everything that has happened at home... it is very hard... so I would rather not talk about it.

A bridging sentence to accommodate the physical examination. You have planned an examination, but during the examination some new areas from the history taking will be opened depending on the findings.

D: Of course. Let's move to the examination couch and check if you are 'broken'. We could also do a general examination, if that is ok?

 

 

 

Observe the set of these techniques in the practical example 'a broken patient'. The first few minutes of the interview has revealed quite a lot of data and quite complex has material come up. Has the focused history been adequate? We can apply the clinical report technique, we can try to synthesise the data we have found: 'a patient without any significant past medical history and with some health promotion data needing completion, comes to the clinic as he thinks he has an inguinal hernia after lifting something heavy a month ago. He has been constipated for the last 3 weeks without any previous history of constipation. He has rectal bleeding in the form of fresh blood around his stools. He thinks both processes are related'. At this point we can see that the chronological characterisation of the blood in the stools has not been sufficiently completed. Missing questions such as: has this happened previously? Is this only recent, as the story seems to infer? Finally, the clinical can plan a problem based physical examination, for example: 'a basic examination needs to be done to this patient to complete the basic data, a superficial and deep examination of his abdomen needs to be performed, including a specific examination to detect an inguinal hernia and I must do a rectal examination looking for blood, anal fissures or haemorrhoids (mostly internal) and tumours'. It is important that each manoeuvre has intentionality: 'in the rectal exam I will look for anal fissures, internal haemorrhoids and tumours'. A hunter does not catch anything if he is not careful, if he cannot imagine the type of movement the brush makes that suggests there is hidden prey. Something similar happens with the physical examination: we are hunting for abnormalities, but to find them we must evoke the image of what we are looking for, even before performing the manoeuvre.

 

Mistakes to avoid

 

We summarise in Table 3.3 the main errors that should be avoided, errors that are discussed below.

 

Table 3.3.

Technical mistakes in the exploratory part

Intuitive interviewers and field dependence.

Focalised interviewers

Blocking patients and interviewers who ask a lot, but... by using closed questions

Skimming too quickly over the psychological aspects

 

 

Intuitive interviewers and field dependence

Please read these definitions carefully: The intuitive interviewer has a tendency to make up data which actually has not been proven sufficiently. The field dependent interviewer: his attention floats over materials that come out in conversation, without following an interview plan. Unfortunately, both syndromes can often co-exist in the same person, giving a typical pattern of dispersed interviewer. For example:

 

P: The itching in my legs doesn't allow me to sleep. I did what you told me and used the glycerine soap I also wash my clothes with this soap, but it has made no difference at all...

I: Does by chance the itching get worse with temperature changes?

P: I think is the bed... I get into the bed and the itching starts... could it be fleas?

I (loses his interview plan and falls into the field dependence): Do you have pets?

P: We had to give away a cat years ago because it gave me asthma.

I (field dependence again): But now you no longer have asthma symptoms, do you?

 

Respecting the will of the patient is an objection which is often used as a reason not to follow the interview plan. However, our goal is to obtain good data for our brain in order to think better and to be able to make better decisions. There is only one situation when is better to be dependent on the field: when we want our interviewee to feel comfortable, Shea (2002) has named this strategy 'feeding the homeless'. In the sense of wandering around with the patient, in an unhurried manner, seeing the material they select. It is an appropriate strategy to meet a suspicious or defensive patient, if we have an interview plan as a reference.

 

Focalised interviewers

These are interviewers who contemplate the spectrum of health-disease from a psychosocial perspective or a biological perspective, but have difficulty in integrating the two perspectives. They are unable to take an extended history, that is, history taking that considers not only the package we call focused history taking, but also the other two packages: the psychosocial and the patient-centred packages. We distinguish three types of focalisations:

 

  • Biological focus: Everything psychological remains in a residual category typed as: 'anxious-depressive', 'functional' or worse still, 'hysterical'. A biologically-focused professional will believe that, first and foremost, the 'organic' cause must be ruled out, and then we will consider the psychological 'by exclusion' of other causes. It is very common that in a patient who complains of 'feeling dizzy' we check their blood pressure, we perform otoscopy, fundoscopy and neurological examination... but nobody asks how their mood is! In table 3.1 we illustrate some questions for what we call the psychosocial jump.

  • Psychological focus: A psychological focus consists of attributing the causes of suffering exclusively to psychosocial factors, and the search is focused on this field. Medical professionals often forget the psychosocial aspects of the illness (Engel GL, 1977; 1980), while nurses have, on occasions, the opposite trend. A dermatological problem can be interpreted as 'poor hygiene', high blood pressure readings or blood sugar levels to 'she is always nervous', etc. While a patient often excuses a professional who confuses their depression with osteoarthritis, it they do not often forgive the opposite mistake. The verb 'balint' has been coined (verb derived from Balint, in allusion to the well-known psychiatrist, Gask L, 1988; Aseguinolaza L, 2000), to refer to the following scenario: 'A complicated patient in whom a concrete disease or a lasting relief weren't found, that after empathic listening, the patient shares a relevant intrapersonal conflict; after sharing this conflict, their symptoms disappear'. Without a doubt, some patients may respond to this scheme, but they are very few. In general, we can say that a purely psychological approach means ignoring relevant organic causes of the disease. Imagine patients with hypothyroidism, with their tiredness, aches and sleep disorder, being subjected to a third degree cross-examination in search of the conflict they have to verbalise to begin to heal. Everyone has some degree of conflict or psychosocial stress, so these kind of connections are plausible if the interviewer perseveres with them, and with impressionable patients you can cause them to refer to sexual abuse that has not happened (so called "false memories of abuse', Ratey JJ, 2002, p. 270).

  • Symptoms focus: it takes only one question such as: 'are you happy with what you are studying?', 'how is your mother feeling?' for the person to come into the consultation instead of the disease. Gross DA (1998) found more satisfaction in interviews where some social interaction was produced. However, despite requiring a minimum of effort, many interviewers prefer to remain in the tangle of symptoms appreciating only exploring things on a superficial level.

 

So, and as a practical conclusion of the foregoing up to here, how should one proceed to a collection of data that has enough bio-psychosocial 'extension'?:

 

  1. Practice the 'vanishing point' of the interview and the 'emptying of pre-made information'.

  2. Be aware of the first hypotheses that occur to you. Practice reframing to doubt this, with the 'reverse hypothesis 'technique': 'I'm considering a hypothesis of a biological nature... and if the problem was in the psychosocial sphere?' Or vice versa.

  3. Bring the 'whole person' into the consultation. Although you only need to ask a question concerning interests, hobbies or family... do it!

 

Blocking patients and interviewers who ask a lot but... by using close questions!

The natural tendency of any interviewer is to use closed questions, i.e. questions that can be answered with a 'yes' or a 'no', and to focus their attention on the aspects of the presenting complaint, which can be solved more easily. This combination leads to induced interrogations: we take the 'clearer' symptoms and we forget the most vague and difficult to work with.

 

The less effort a patient has to make to answer a question, less reliable the answer. It may also be that the patient answers 'yes' or 'no' to a closed question with the secret hope of pleasing the interviewer. Avoid this trend by including sentences such as: 'tell me more', 'what else happened?', etc in your repertoire of skills.

 

Skimming too quickly over the psychological aspects

Respect the patient’s psychological defences. Note the aggressive style of the interviewer:

 

P: I have come about this headache. I think it comes from my neck.

I (making a premature emotional signal): You look worried, even sad.

P: No at all, I feel fine.

I (Again using an emotional signal, followed by an interpretation): I note you close your hand and you frown as if you are experiencing a lot of tension.

 

In terms of emotions, everything has its own pace. Cautious approaches have advantages:

 

I (while examining the previous patient): I see your muscles are very tense...

P: I’m always told this, but I'm fine.

I (suggested interpretation): Sometimes stress, or everyday problems, show themselves in our backs...

P: I don’t think so. I have this headache and it is not related to stress.

I (giving in): of course, your headache. Let's take your blood pressure...

 

Although we have a strong suspicion that there are psychosocial elements influencing the patient's symptoms, we will have to delay addressing them until the patient opens the door slightly.

 

Situations Gallery

In this section we will examine:

 

  • The vague patient.

  • Difficulties getting into the psychosocial aspects.

  • Starting from scratch!

  • Pelvic examination.

  • History taking of sexual behaviour and risk assessment.

 

The vague patient

Assume that the doctor has already introduced himself and properly greeted the patient when this scene begins.

 

Abbreviations: D: doctor; P: patient.

/1/P: Doctor, I wake up very often during the night to pass urine.

/2/D: Do you have a burning sensation when you pass urine?

/3/P: No, what happens is that I get nervous and I have to get up.

/4/D: And why are you nervous?

/5/P: I don't have any reason to be nervous... I think I wake up because I feel some nerves in my legs and if I don't get up... I explode

/6/D: So, you come today because you don't sleep well at night, do you?

/7/P: No, I come today because I have to go to get up at night very often to pass urine.

/8/D: And then do you pass a lot of urine?

/9/P: Sometimes only a little bit, that is why I am surprised.

/10/D: And sometimes you don’t even pass urine?

/11/P: Yes, I do.

/12/D (irritated): So what a mess, there is no clarification.

 

Comments

1. Has the doctor any reason to complain about this patient who is seemingly confused in their responses?

Doctors have an idealised image of how a good patient should be. We would like the patient to understand our haste and collaborate by providing reliable and ordered data. When the opposite happens, we cannot avoid feelings of irritability, and we easily think: 'this patient is so annoying' rather than think: 'the data I need to get a good diagnosis is here, but it depends on my ability to know how to get it. Patients do not have any obligation to be the good patients that practitioners want, and instead this idea of a good patient harms the interviewer who holds it. The right question is not: 'why isn’t this patient a good patient?', but: 'what should I do to accommodate this patient?’

 

2. Does the doctor make any mistakes? If so, which ones?

The interviewer still corresponds to a field-dependent style. As previously described: field dependent interviewers set the questions and the interview plan in general, from data provided by the patient at each point of the interview without generating the necessary hypotheses to 'detach from' the symptomatic field and to create a proper interview plan.

 

3. Does the Doctor’s clarification of the presenting complaint in /6 /seem correct to you?

/6/D: So, you come today because you don't sleep well at night, do you?

 

Clarifications always help though, as in this case, they are not formed in a particularly skilled manner. They help because they force the clinician and patient to agree on the material they are going to work with together.

It is preferable to obtain a 'no' or a rectification from the patient to an inaccurate clarification, than a forced agreement.

 

4. Could you think of any possible diagnosis which could justify this patient's presenting complaint?

If the doctor applied the textual listening technique (read what they have just heard as if it were written and belonged to an anonymous patient), he would obtain the following semiological data:

 

  • This is a 55 year old lady who has to get up at night to urinate.

  • She describes feeling a sensation of 'nerves in the legs' which force her to get up, and if she doesn't do this she 'explodes'

  • There aren’t any clear urinary symptoms being described.

 

 If the doctor 'had read' this data, as now you do, instead of 'having heard' it in a slightly confused context, he would certainly consider the following possibilities:

 

  • Restless legs syndrome in a person who additionally suffers from urinary frequency of another origin.

  • An initial presentation of heart failure with a degree of nocturnal paroxysmal dyspnoea, which the patient confuses with restlessness

  • Anxiety which the patient somatises as an urgency to urinate.

 

We mention these possibilities, but undoubtedly there are more. What interests us here is the blockade to the patient’s story permitted by the clinician, and their anger at the patient's seemingly confused style. This irritation leads the clinician to trivialise the patient’s symptoms (‘these complaints are nonsense').

 

What must we do in such situations?

1. There cannot be a good interview without defining the presenting complaint well. Direct the interview towards achieving this, even if you have to interrupt a very talkative patient. The patient will accept the interruption and restarting the interview if the clinician does not show signs of nervousness or irritation. More specifically, the doctor will be able to 'draw' a map of complaints, which is, to explore all presenting complaints from the patient without ranking them, and to allow the patient to suggest to you the reason for consultation. For example:

 

/4/D: Ok, and is there anything else worrying you?

 

Opening the map of complaints allows the doctor to use it as a symptom-guide to choose the one that appears to be the most relevant.

 

2. Let it be the patient be who chooses the importance of their symptoms. Sometimes it can be useful to say: 'Of all the things which you have told me about today, which has brought you in today?', or even, when the patient raises some difficult personal problems to solve we can try: 'what do you think I can do to help you?'

 

3. Clarify: 'what do you understand about...?' Another similar technique is the forced clarification: 'please, could you try to explain your dizziness without using the word dizziness?’

 

4. A very useful technique is creating scenarios: 'How would you feel if you had to stand up?', 'What would happen if you had to walk a hill?' 'Let's imagine you find the shopping trolley and had to go up to the top floor', etc.

 

5. Menu of suggestions: the discomfort you feel... is it a sharp pain or a feeling of pressure?

 

6. When a patient expresses their self in very unclear way, or you have the feeling of 'being completely lost', consider the possibility of starting from scratch (as if you didn't know the patient) and apply the 'map of demands and complaints', 'pathobiography' and 'textual reading' techniques. In this situation, relax, take your time and let your mind work freely. Do not force yourself to 'demonstrate' that ' you were right', but quite the opposite: 'and what if I was going down the wrong path until now?' (This technique is named the 'reverse hypothesis technique'). Try to be a good listener to the echoes which the patient's words evoke in your head. How many times do clinicians recognize in hindsight that, at some point, we considered the correct diagnosis, but discarded it because we considered it to be 'unlikely'!

 

7. In elderly patients and patients who are cognitively impaired, the help of a family member or a carer may be essential to enable the clinician to understand exactly what happens, what are their daily activities, whether they take their medication or not. This should be a team task. Sometimes our principal task consists of mobilising family resources so the patient is cared for in their family environment.

 

We’re going to implement some of these ideas in the example above. Interestingly, the case is very easy to solve simply with the ‘repetition of words or phrases’ technique, which allows us to clarify the content of what the patient said:

 

/1/P: Doctor, I wake up at night very often to urinate.

/2/D: I see... Tell me more about that

/3/P: I get nervous and I have to get up.

/4/D: Are you nervous?

/5/P: I don’t have any reason to be nervous. What happens is that I wake up because I feel some nerves in my legs and if I don't get up... I explode

/6/D: I understand...if you don't wake up, you feel you would explode?

/7/P: The feeling I have in my legs means I can’t hold them still. Touching the cold floor is the only thing which relieves the feeling.

 

At this point is becoming quite clear that the patient describes restless legs syndrome, and that the confusion came from the relationship established with urination.

Also note these skills in another fragment:

 

/1/P: I feel dizzy; this tiredness is actually getting worse. I get up from the chair and I feel dizzy, I have palpitations and I feel short of breath too. I feel scared even walking into the corridor, as I know all my symptoms will get worse.

/2/I (clarifying): What do you mean by dizziness?

/3/P: When I wake up in the morning and when I get up from the chair, I start feeling the dizziness again.

/4/I (forced clarification): Try to explain the feelings you have when getting out of bed without using the word dizziness. (Creating a scenario) You get up from your bed and...

/5/P: Well, I get up and I lose my sight and then I feel sick! I say to myself, 'calm down, and don’t move your head' and then I seem to recover a bit.

/6/I: Can you reach the toilet without help?

/7/P: Yes, but holding onto the walls and walking little by little

/8/I (menu of suggestions): Do you feel weak or do things spin around you?

/9/P: As if I am losing sight of the world. Really really bad dizziness

 

In just a few minutes of interview we have achieved relevant information about the patient’s symptoms, which guides us towards positional dizziness. However, several symptoms have appeared and we follow them: a) breathlessness on exertion; b) an increasing dizziness when the patient leaves the room. The initial data points towards heart or respiratory failure, the second part points towards symptoms of anxiety. We must follow each one of these possibilities.

 

 

In summary, in front of a vague patient:


  • Delineate the presenting complaint and make a map of complaints.

  • Let the patient talk, but clarify what they are saying.

  • Classify the data as if you had to write a medical report.

  • 'Read' the data from the patient as if they were 'another' patient.


 

 

Difficulties getting into the psychosocial aspects

We have previously referred to two types of very common difficulties in the exploratory part of the interview: a) drawing the chronology of the symptoms sufficiently, and b) addressing the psychosocial aspects. These difficulties are reflected in the following example:

 

Dr.: How can I help you today?

P: A get a very severe dizziness from time to time. I think I may have something in my ears.

Dr.: Is there anything else?

P: No, just this. But when I get feel dizzy, it is very strong, I almost fall over, I have to support myself, and then it disappears. I get very scared.

Dr.: Have you noticed any hearing loss?

P: No, no.

Dr.: And double vision? Or headaches?

P: No, nothing like that...

Dr.: Have you had any ringing in your ears?

P: No.

Dr.: Ok, do you mind getting on the couch? I would like to examine you.

 

Comments

1. Have characteristics of the symptoms been sufficiently established?

The interviewer ignores important aspects such as: when and how the symptoms started, what exactly the patient means by a sensation of dizziness, what has happened to the symptoms over time, what makes the dizziness worse or better, what the patient relates the symptoms to and what their beliefs and expectations are about these. Strangely, this type of short history taking is performed by professionals who have many years of clinical practice. It is as if by knowing the presenting complaint and the patient's appearance, they already knew what happens to the patient (intuitive interviewer).

 

2. When can we be sure that we have made taken a sufficient history?

We talked previously about the 'medical report technique'. It consists of asking yourself: 'with the data I have, could I make a clinical report about what is happening to this patient?' Try to document in the medical records a report which includes the how, when and where of the patient’s symptoms and you will immediately detect your gaps.

 

3. Is it important to address the psychosocial aspects?

It is very important. In a large proportion of patients suffering from dizziness, their symptoms are due to psychosocial processes such as: anxiety, depression, adaptive disorders, etc. The patient's hypothesis of, 'I think I have something in my ears', can make it difficult for the clinician to explore the psychosocial aspects. In any case, we must learn to distinguish the symptoms which could be due to an underlying psychosocial cause, and not hesitate to make the psychosocial leap.

 

4. How can we get into the psychosocial aspects without raising psychological resistance?

Some patients react badly when they are asked: 'Are you somewhat more anxious?', and especially if they are told directly: 'All this is anxiety'. Questions related to the 'psycho-social package' might be more acceptable to the patient if we start by saying something like: 'How do you rest at night? Or: 'how is your mood?' Although we do this, we could precipitate resistance like in the case we will analyse next.

 

What must we do in such situations?

Let's look at the example above, but selecting the moment when the professional makes the psychosocial leap:

 

Dr. (summarising): if I understood correctly, you feel sudden dizziness, lasting just a few seconds, as if you are going to lose your balance. It happens anywhere and it has been going on for 2 months. Is this correct?

P: Yes, it is.

Dr. (psychosocial leap): Has your sleep become worse?

P: Yes, it has.

Dr. (After finding more details about the patient’s insomnia, they continue by saying) how is your mood at the moment?

P: No, this is not related to stress, because I feel fine and not anxious.

Dr. (facing psychological resistance to explore the 'mind'): I am not saying this is anxiety, but mood is one more aspect of humans, the desire to do things, hopes you may have right now... All those things also interest me. What about that?

P: I don't have many hopes right now.

Dr. (making an emotional signalling): You are saying this as if you were sad...

P (tearful): As you would expect me to be since my wife has left me after 15 years of marriage!

 

Resistance to tackling the psychological aspects doesn't mean a prohibition. In fact, patients showing more resistance tend to be those who need this type of approach the most. It is a painful step which must be handed with tact and an appropriate technique and skill.

 

 

 

Remember, to do the psycho-social leap:


  • Start by finding out about the patient’s sleep pattern as this rarely elicits resistance. If you discover they are having nightmares, it may have direct relationship to the patient’s most hidden anxieties.

  • Continue with enquiring about the patient’s mood, hopes and concerns.

  • Avoid attributing symptoms to the 'nerves'. Choose diagnostic terms such as 'anxiety', 'stress' or other terms more suited to the clinical picture.

  • If the patient is reluctant to discuss the psychosocial elements, justify your interest in these aspects as a means to get to know them as a person, not just as a patient.


 

 

Starting from scratch!

Knowing patients for years undoubtedly has advantages, but it also leads to pathology of familiarity. First of all, seeing a patient regularly doesn't mean that we know them. It may well happen that all or almost all of the previous encounters were carried out on a superficial level, without a preventive assessment or a systems review. Secondly, we experience a false sense of security, as if by the fact of the patient having visited many times we have averted the worst case scenarios. This false security is increased with hypochondriacal patients in whom we have repeatedly excluded cancer. There comes a time in which this aphorism is true: 'you come to see me so much that I disregard you'. Thirdly, we process a lot of information provided by the patient as being 'already known' and do not consider it. We divide the profile of complaints into those 'already addressed in the past', and we selectively attend to the 'new' complaints. It is a good strategy if we are scrupulous and write down the exact profile of presenting complaints. There is a difference between the patient who presents with a loss of appetite, and only that, and the patient who is additionally losing weight. In fact, we must periodically reprocess the symptoms which may change in severity or intensity. Remember that a picture of diffuse body pain can precede the onset of cancer (McBeth J, 2003).

 

Observe the following situation:

 

P: I can't continue like this! Please take away this excruciating pain or refer me somewhere...

I: I don't understand, what pain are you talking about?

P: Pain in my whole body, you cannot imagine how much it hurts.

I: (Reviewing his medical records): Let's see, I can see previous visits about your high blood pressure, diabetes and knee arthritis, but you have never complained of pain in your whole body before.

P: I mentioned it on previous occasions and you ignored me.

I: It is hard for me to have ignored a complaint which you have only told me about for the first time today, don't you think?

 

Comments:

1. Which is the best strategy in this type of interview?

The best is to start as if we are seeing the patient for the first time, with the idea of forgetting what we know, any prejudice, and any feelings of irritation towards the patient. 'Not knowing always bothers us, but not knowing in the context of a patient who has come to see us in regularly in recent weeks, tends to irritate us. Starting from scratch is an exercise in true humility!

 

2. What does 'starting from scratch' really mean?

It means reviewing patient's medical records, verifying and completing generic data, particularly the family and psychological history, and start taking a new history at least on the current complaints. The pathobiography technique we saw in Chapter 2 can be of great help.

 

What must we do in such situations?

In the following situation the patient came seven days ago with acute bronchitis that has improved. Today he comes for review of his sick note.

 

I: How do you feel Mr Wright?

P: Bad. I feel very weak. I think that this time the bronchitis is very bad because I have to drag myself out of bed.

 

After physical examination:

 

I: I can see that your bronchitis has improved a lot.

P: But I don't feel hungry and I am always sick. I am losing weight. I am not ready to go back to work.

I: Ok, I will do a sick note for another week for you to recover.

 

Seven days after the following conversation occurs:

 

I: How are you feeling Mr Wright? Have you recovered now?

P: No, not at all. You didn’t listen to me, and I've already told you that I feel very bad.

I: Do you have a cough or are you bringing up any phlegm...?

P: No, it’s not the cough which making me weak and is stopping me working. I am not working and am weak because I have lost 10 kilos in a week.

I: Ten kilos in a week? This is ridiculous, it is completely impossible...

P: Look at how loose my trousers are to see if it is impossible...

 

At this point the interviewer literally does not know where to start, so he decides to start from scratch. After reviewing the patient’s family and past medical history, he addresses his current disease again, but from a new perspective.

 

I: (Implementing the technique of visualisation chronology-plot-impact): Let's see, Mr. Wright, I would like to start from the beginning as if I’m seeing you for the first time. When did you start feeling unwell?

P: In fact, for the last month I have felt my digestion is slow... Yes, I have not been eating well or feeling well after eating for a month.

I: And then you had bronchitis.

P: That's right, but I would say that I was already feeling weak... could it be that my defences are low?

I (Ignoring the patient's questions and performing a summary): Allow me to continue: if I have understood correctly, you have lost your appetite and some weight over the last month... is that correct?

P: I guess so. When I came with bronchitis, I asked for some blood tests and a chest x-ray, but you said it was not worth doing.

I (Ignoring the accusation): Indeed, about 15 days ago you started coughing, having a fever and feeling more fatigued, is this right?... And when you recovered from the cough, you’ve still lost your appetite, and food, you say is disgusting...

P: Exactly, I can't eat anything. I've been eating soups and juices for the last 10 days... could this be due to the antibiotics I was taking? But this was also happening before I took them.

 

The interviewer continues with his task to establish that the patient has been off colour for a month on which a self-limiting acute bronchitis is added. Later investigations show the patient has stomach cancer which is beginning to invade the portal space. If the doctor had been stubborn in his first hypothesis ('this patient only wants to lengthen his sick note'), he would have delayed the diagnosis with consequent blame from the patient: « you have 'you told me I had bronchitis and actually I had cancer'.

 

 

 

Remember, in the face of an unclear clinical picture:


  • Start as if you do not knew the patient.

  • Practice the technique of ‘visualisation'.

  • Obvious as it may seem, be wary of any clinical picture, which has an important impact on the patient's life. Do not attribute secondary gain as the cause of the clinical picture until you have completed an exhaustive study.


 

 

Pelvic examination

In a culture of modesty, the pelvic examination is sometimes delayed to avoid making the patient feel uncomfortable. This is a big mistake. We should normalise this type of examination: 'we routinely do rectal examinations on people of your age', 'this examination is quick and is at least as important as an X-ray', etc. Observe how this practice nurse introduces a pelvic examination:

 

 

N: We will now proceed to a pelvic exam. In your case I will need to make a pelvic examination with my fingers first to see if there is a problem with your womb. Sometimes a problem occurs in this part and we need to do a therapeutic curettage

P: I had a scraping when I had an abortion.

N: Then you know what I am talking about. Anyway, as you are not asleep now, after doing a pelvic examination with my fingers, I will introduce the speculum gently, and I will open it to see the cervix... you have had this done before, haven't you?

P: Yes, for a smear test.

N: I see, so you know it is not painful and it allows us to rule out any cancer or infection and to take a sample and analyse it in order to further check there is not a problem. Actually, I think that the problem may be in your womb, but while we are examining you, we can do a smear test.

 

Comments

1 Do you think there are any mistakes or aspects of the interview which could be improved? In general, the interviewer was very correct, but we could object to her saying:

 

'Sometimes a problem occurs in this part and we need to do a therapeutic curettage'. At this point she is suggesting hypothetical events.

'So you know it is not painful and it allows us to rule out any cancer or infection'. The word cancer has a high emotional content and it should be avoided. The patient may well think: 'I am having this test as they suspect I have cancer'.

And later: 'I think that the problem may be in your womb, but while we are examining you, we can do a smear test'.

 

It is inappropriate to give uncertain information (there is still no diagnosis and when we have a diagnosis, this should be given by the doctor), but it is also inappropriate when she says 'while we are examining you', as it implies we are carrying out a somewhat secondary test.

 

How should we act in such situations?

With assertion. If you do not give importance to modesty, the patient won’t either. For example:

 

I (Rationality of the examination): We need to do a rectal examination to examine your prostate... (Establishing bidirectionality) Do you know what this examination involves?

P: No, I have never had it before. But my father suffers from prostate cancer.

I (converts the fear into preventive action): This is all the more reason to assess your prostate. The examination consists of feeling the prostate by inserting a finger into the anus... is not painful provided that you are relaxed... Please move to the couch and we will prepare you...

 

Let's imagine that at this point the patient exhibits resistance and we consider this examination absolutely necessary:

 

P: I would rather not to do this.

I: (Finding out the patient's beliefs): Why is that? Is it fear, embarrassment or would you like to go to the bathroom first?

P: A bit of everything....

 

The patient has not organized their thoughts. At this time we can opt to work through them a little bit more, applying motivational interviewing strategies, and even delay the examination by booking another appointment. But imagine that it is a test that we cannot postpone:

 

I: In your case this test should not be postponed. (Normalises it) This test is something we do very frequently. (Legitimises it) It is normal to feel embarrassed, but think that for us it is something we are used to doing. (Favouring the patient’s control over the situation) If you would like to go to the toilet and come back in a few minutes it is not a problem, but I recommend that you do it now, although you don’t feel prepared. These things are often better if you don’t think about them too much, don't you agree?

 

Avoid saying: 'if you prefer to come back another day, there is no rush...' as the patient may not come back. Do not mind being a bit insistent, because although the patient can consider you 'annoying' at the time, the prevailing thought will eventually be 'luckily I had the test done'.

 

 

Remember, overcoming the culture of modesty means:


  • Be respectful of the patient's privacy, place a sheet or paper towel over their intimate parts, preserve their space with a curtain, etc.

  • Ask for consent for students to be present.

  • Explain the test and normalise their modesty or embarrassment.

  • Be very clear regarding the need for the examination: 'In the long run, this exam needs to be done, and it is better to do it now than later'.


 

 

Taking a history of sexual behaviour and risk assessment

To finish this situations gallery, we have chosen taking a sexual behaviour and risk assessment because of its importance. The techniques that we will see are similar to the ones we have to use to take a history of other behaviours (diet history, social history, etc.), with the exception that we are entering an area where we will have to build up the patient's trust. Note the following interview with a 24-year-old man:

 

I: Before proceeding to address your cold, I would be like to speak to you a bit about risk behaviour. Are you gay or straight?

P: I don't understand...

I: Do you have sex with men or women?

P (Laughing): Mostly with the TV.

I: Do you masturbate?

P: Whenever I am watching Heidi TV series..

I (Confused): Well, I suppose you must have porn videos and such ... right?

 

The patient does not respond, looking at the interviewer with a mocking smile. The interviewer changes the subject.

 

Comments

1. What was the error made by this interviewer?

The interviewer introduces the subject abruptly, which makes the patient defensive. He may have become annoyed by being suddenly asked about his sexual orientation and he has retaliated by mocking the interviewer. At this point the clinician doesn't know how to continue and changes the topic.

 

2. How can we introduce the topic of sexuality without causing tension? Some patients tolerate, and even like being asked directly, but in general it is preferable to use a graded approach. For example:

 

  • Do you have a boyfriend/girlfriend, friend or partner?

  • How is your intimate relationship with your partner?

  • Sometimes chronic illnesses can cause sexual difficulties, have you noticed any problems?

  • Have you noticed any changes in your libido?

  • Many people today day are concerned about getting HIV; do you have any concerns about this or any risk of infection? (After the patient's answer the interviewer clarifies): As well as contracting it from a sexual relationship between a man and a woman by having intercourse without a condom, receiving a blood transfusion, sharing needles, and men, who have sexual relationships with men, are the most important risk factors... are you in any of these groups?

 

What must we do in such situations?

We must always try to earn the patient's trust with a gradual introduction to the subject, standardizing our questioning and moving from general questions on the subject to specific questions. In the following case, we are speaking to a 56-year-old woman. We suspect that there is a possible sexual problem that may be influencing her long-standing dysthymia.

 

I: (Making a summary of the information obtained so far): I can see that your difficulties haven’t changed from what you told me three months ago. However, I would like to move a little further forward today. Something that we have never addressed is whether you are satisfied with your relationship with your husband.

P: Well, once I told you that my husband keeps things to himself. He does not communicate. He watches TV, he reads things related to football, but he talks... little and badly.

I  (Screening for domestic violence): have you ever felt threatened?

P: No, never. I don't know how to say this, he isn’t a very affectionate person, but he has never been violent to me.

I (Suggested interpretation): If I clarify, your complaint would be rather a lack of affection...

P: Yes, it is.

I: And on a more personal level, is there an intimate relationship?

P: No, because he is unable to talk about serious topics.

I (He realises that the patient hasn't understood, so he clarifies the question): I mean what is your sexual relationship with your partner like...

P: For at least three years he hasn't asked me anything. Sometimes I tell him 'we are very young to be celibate, why don't you go to the doctor?' But he doesn't want to discuss it. He says that his hernia prevents it. You tell me...

 

Don't forget the golden rule: For sexual and risk behaviours we always need to be very clear. We can start the dialogue in a metaphorical way, but if we have to ask a second time we must make our communication as clear as possible.

 

Remember, for risk behaviours:


  • Start history taking the history in a way that doesn't cause friction: 'what do you drink while having lunch?' or if assessing suicide risk: 'do you think that life is worth living?’

  • While having a conversation, be very clear: 'how many liqueur coffees do you have in the morning?' or: 'have you made any plans to harm yourself?’

  • At some point comment: 'It is very common to talk about/examine this, don't worry'. What is normal for you will seem normal to the patient.


 

 

Advanced Concepts

Generally, the different phases of the interview analysed up to here take a very short amount of time. We greet the patient, we find out the presenting complaint, and in less than a minute we proceed to the verbal exploration. In the initial moments of the interview, the interviewer will have proceeded to obtain the readymade information, and apply techniques of narrative support. From the initial information; the interviewer will have generated their first hypothesis trying to answer two important questions: 'What is happening to the patient?', and 'what am I expected to do?’ This is the framing or the interview's intention. The interviewer will start making assumptions in an automatic way (hypothesis generating), and from them they will produce an interview plan (Burack RC, 1983; Esposito V, 1983; Boucher FG, 1980). In the following pages we will see the necessity of combining these hypotheses and thoughts (advanced interview plan) with a basic interview plan. Following this and using the data we have obtained, we need to apply a process to decide which hypothesis to keep. This is a highly complex process where we combine two types of thought: intuitive thought (or thought guided by analogies and similarities) and thought guided by criteria. Finally, we will examine the bio-psycho-social model and what it brings to the diagnostic task.

 

Drawings in the mind

One of the most important challenges we face in the clinical interview is whether different clinicians obtain a similar diagnosis in the same patient and clinical situation. Studies on the subject are named diagnostic variability studies and they cover the fields of both medicine and nursing. We have the impression that one of the key elements which explains the differences between clinicians when analysing their patients' problems is the way in which they learn, or expressed another way, by the images in their mind of the different problems or clinical situations. Let's briefly examine this point and we will also give some pointers on how we can share these mental drawings more efficiently, which is so important when interpreting the clinical reality.

 

First statement: when we try to understand a clinical situation, we don't use an intuitive method. It is not true that we collect data and a diagnosis or a way of describing the problem just appears in front of us. We always have a few previous schemes or models, which we use to interpret the reality of the situation. Knowledge, as Popper understands, is always reasoned (Popper K, 1972). Due to this, for many years doctors have been applying all kinds of diagnoses to patients with fibromyalgia, and we now probably see patients who in the future we will group under different headings. We only see what we are prepared to see. We only see what we somehow already have in our brain as a model.

 

Second statement: when we see a particular patient we only apply a part of what we know. This paradox is similar to the student that finds a multiple choice exam easier than an exam with short answer questions. In the first case 'how things sound' is more than enough to answer the question; in the second case, the student has to summarise their knowledge to explain it in a reasoned manner. It is another way of studying. Similarly, the clinician has to summarise what they have learnt to use the information and explain this to the patient. As Schon (1987) points out there is a knowledge in action, almost automatic knowledge that we can retrieve in front of the patient; and on the other hand, there are many skills that would be able to retrieve in an exam, but that we are unable to retrieve in front of a patient. Think how much it takes you get used to prescribing a completely new medication out of habit, and how many times you have to look at the BNF to reassure yourself. Or how dangerous is to interpret an x-ray in front of the patient, during the consultation, compared at the end of the clinic and we have our time entirely dedicated to it.

 

There are two consequences of this approach: on the one hand, in relation to how to study, and on the other, to how to plan the interview to get the best result.

 

Study clinic. A way to study clinical skills (keeping with the idea of 'knowledge in action') is the technique we term package to use. Table 3.4 shows an example of a case of fibromyalgia, and how a clinician without expertise performs the clinical reasoning for it, from the literature he reviews. The table is not intended to be a review of the condition, but it illustrates the discursive process we propose

Planning the interview. The second factor we touched on is that we have to give ourselves time to obtain quality data, without rushing into trying to make a diagnosis and 'knowing what to do as soon as possible'. To do this we have to combine a basic plan (the essential tasks), with an advanced plan (tasks that occur on the basis of materials provided by the patient). Let’s look at this in more detail.

 

Table 3.4.

Package to use: fibromyalgia as an example

When I shall suspect it: in cases of diffuse pain, pain without a definite cause, pain which is expressed in a dramatic manner, polymyalgia in patients with insomnia or patients with a weary or depressed appearance.

 

Verification points: pain in 9 of the 18 standard points, with discomfort for at least three months, ask about sleep and mood, but without causing resistance, exclude arthritis, fever and systemic symptoms...

 

Conditions which present similarly: depression or emotional distress, polymyalgia rheumatica, systemic diseases.

In my case, I have the following difficulties with the assessment: I am unclear of the exact location of the fibromyalgia points and the pressure I need to use to assess them. It is urgent that I learn this with an expert. Until this aspect is clear I am not in a position to make this diagnosis.

 

Red flags in communication with the patient: I should avoid frightening the patient by using the word 'fibromyalgia', because the patient may associate this term with a serious illness.

 

Red flags in diagnosis: I should not use the diagnosis before confirming biological normality.

Treatment options: first choice, low dose amitriptyline; second option: muscle relaxants and other selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs).

 

Red flags in treatment: not having in depth knowledge of the use of this drug and how patients should respond to treatment and the side effects. Nor do I know the next step if amitriptyline is not effective. Before proceeding to treat the patient, I must clarify these points.

This example simulates how a clinician who does not know this disease packages the information in order to use it in a specific clinical situation.

 

 

Basic and advanced interview plan

Imagine a clinician trying to discern the nature of chest pain in a 23 years old patient without any risk factors. 'The pain goes from my stomach, up to my chest and it feels very tight, I am worried it is my heart' says the young man. How does the clinician rule this out?

 

On one hand, he has a basic interview plan that must be completed: 'active listening', a 'focused history' package, and a basic physical examination. But as he progresses in the focused history package, diagnostic hypotheses occur to him: 'and if he had oesophagitis?' This scenario raises the question: 'when you go to sleep or you lie down, do you feel acid coming up into your mouth?' Therefore, we construct our listening and our questions from two types of leads:

 

  • A very generic lead (the left part of Figure 3.2) which indicates large areas and issues that need to be addressed: how, when and where of the pain, factors that exacerbate or relieve it, the patient's beliefs, etc.

  • A specific lead to diagnostic hypotheses which appear as being plausible. For example: 'this pain could be due to pericarditis, therefore I have to ask, etc.'

 

Figure 3.2 shows how combining the basic and advanced plans leads to the interview finally carried out.

 

Therefore, the interview plan is changing at every bend in the road, depending on both the verbal exploration and the exploratory data arising. And here is where a good clinician overcomes three pitfalls to produce the data.

 

Fig-3.2

Figure 3.2.

Basic and advanced interview plan: verification points.

 

 

Three difficulties in interpreting and formulating clinical data. Concept of critical stress

There are three obstacles we must overcome when we produce the data which we have collected in the history taking and physical examination (Table 3.5).

 

Table 3.5.

The three obstacles to making a good interview plan

  • Reconcile and complete the basic interview plan with the advanced interview plan. Avoid insights and field dependent trends.

  • Perform reframing of the first hypothesis. Overcome your reluctance and laziness in order to 'rethink' the patient.

  • Delay the resolution of the interview. Withstand critical tension.

 

 

Firstly, we have to avoid the temptation of 'taking shortcuts', either via intuition or by forgetting (in the example above, we may forget about the chest pain if we become too focused on the epigastric pain). These shortcuts sometimes lead us to not complete the basic plan; for example, we find tonsils covered in pus and then do not examine the chest.

 

A second pitfall consists of overcoming our laziness, in order to deny the first hypothesis. In the case below (Figure 3.2) the first hypotheses are: The patient's anxiety or apprehension. But on performing the basic examination, the doctor finds hepatomegaly. Is the doctor required to modify his first hypothesis? That always represents a major effort. This crosses his mind: 'Am I in the presence of two diseases, chest pain and hepatomegaly? Or only one disease? For example: Alcoholism which has resulted in anxiety and various somatisations?' But he also thinks: 'maybe the hepatomegaly does not have any significance, and if I ignore it, it will save me a lot of effort'. There is always a dormant desire to end the interview as soon as possible.

 

Do we always want to end the interview 'as soon as possible'? Yes, almost always. The exploratory part of the interview accumulates too much tension: we don't know what is wrong with the patient; we do not know what we need to tell them, if they will accept our explanation or if we will address their expectations. Consequently, the clinical act always moves in the field of emotions, and generically, in the area of stress. We experience some tension (we do not know what is wrong with the patient or what to tell them); we want to offload our stress (by issuing a diagnosis, or saying what should be done). When the patient pushes us, even more stress builds up, for example:

 

P: You don't know what it is happening to me?

I: So then, I am going to refer you to a colleague to see if they know what is going on.

 

This doctor offloads the tension with the easiest answer. In Figure 3.3 we illustrate 'endurance' to stress with the 'critical stress threshold 2' line. The line located above ('critical stress threshold 1') shows a professional with more emotional control, less irritable, more tolerant of the ambiguity of 'not knowing'.

 

Therefore, in Table 3.5 the two cognitive difficulties: a) reconciling the general interview plan with a specific plan to the emerging interview in a dynamic way, and b) allowing a second hypothesis (reframing), leads us to a third obstacle of an emotional nature: c) to tolerate an interview which becomes complicated and delay its resolution in order to obtain better data. In summary: to tolerate a higher critical stress. Hence, we promote a model of the clinical act which is emotional (tolerates a critical stress) and simultaneously cognitive or rational (to rationally develop the clinical data).

 

The concept of critical stress threshold explains what we have called the approach or interview in two phases. Once we have ruled out an 'organic' cause in a patient with somatic symptoms, we arrange another interview in order to fill in the psychosocial data. A psycho-social diagnosis doesn't activate 'stress' in the same way as a biological diagnosis does, and it is postponed by the interviewer.

 

Fig-3.3

Figure 3.3.

Critical stress threshold

 

Each interviewer has a capacity for tolerating uncertainty and 'not knowing what to do'. The capacity for tolerating this is named the critical stress threshold. Reaching this threshold activates resolution of the interview, although the way in which the interview is resolved is not appropriate. In the example if the interviewer had a higher critical stress threshold (threshold 2); he would not terminate the interview in such an abrupt manner.

 

 

Making sense of the patient's story. The conditions which are sufficient to enable a diagnosis.

The clinician constantly asks their self: 'Am I going forward in understanding what happens to my patient?', and also: 'how long do I have to 'solve this case'?'. Interviewers are very different when deciding when to finish taking the history. Some clinicians are content with a few statements and have already formed a hypothesis or an assumption about what happens to the patient. Others, however, are meticulous and are perfectionists. The former quickly say to themselves... 'I have it. I know what happens to this patient'. Others are less hurried. In any case, we have sufficient conditions to establish a diagnosis. In a patient complaining about nocturnal tingling of both hands, this information alone is not enough to diagnose carpal tunnel syndrome. In general, we make a compromise between verbal information and exploratory data. On the other hand, each entity has its own sufficiency conditions which may change depending on the clinical situation. For example: 'if I go on a home visit to a patient I would consider a fever, with crackles on auscultation and pleuritic pain to be sufficient data to establish a possible diagnosis of pneumonia. On the other hand, in a hospital emergency room, I will require a chest X-ray which is compatible with such a diagnosis'.

 

But good intentions are one thing, but putting this into good clinical practice is another thing. The clinician, as we said, is always looking for 'shortcuts'. Such an approach is what we call the 'one plus one law'. It consists of the following: if the information provided by the patient orients you towards a particular diagnosis, say, carpal tunnel syndrome ('I have a tingling sensation in my hand during the night'), and then the clinician elicits a positive test for carpal tunnel syndrome (e.g., Phalen's test) they will give a diagnosis of carpal tunnel syndrome. Figure 3.4 illustrates the one plus one law, precipitating the 'I know it' or the 'I already know what happens to this patient', with a premature closure of the interview.

 

Fig-3.4

Figure 3.4

One plus one law and closing interview.

It is taken as a certainty finding some exploratory data or data in the history taking which points in the same direction as a hypothesis we have just thought of. When this circumstance occurs, in many occasions, the clinician then fails to execute manoeuvres of the basic interview plan.

 

ACE inhibitor: Angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitor

 

 

The concept of critical stress (figure 3.3) helps us understand the one plus one law. The less we tolerate the emotional stress of 'not knowing', the more we look for shortcuts and the more we rely on intuitive thinking. And conversely, when we tolerate uncertainty, we can be much more detailed, and we can use thought based on criteria much more. To allow our brains to perform clinical reasoning based on criteria, we should allow a certain amount of time to process the information first. Many clinicians think: 'well, first I will listen to my intuition, and if that doesn't work, I will rethink the case from a more thorough approach'. Over the years, this is the natural tendency of all clinicians. The trap is if the clinician does not have the mental agility to reframe things, often they won't want to move from their first diagnostic orientation. They remain 'anchored' in their first diagnostic hypothesis, without assuming the emotional cost of reframing, and gradually impoverish their thought based on criteria. Before proceeding in the analysis of thought by criteria, let's look a little more at the concept of anchoring the diagnosis.

 

Anchoring the diagnosis

We define anchoring the diagnosis as the degree of certainty or social commitment we have with a particular diagnosis. Certainty is generally due to evidence or scientific tests (exploratory data, analysis, scan results). The commitment, on the other hand, has an emotional and social origin. We previously saw that an intuitive doctor wants to prove he has 'a good clinical eye'. For him to reframe a diagnosis it may cause him to believe his is 'being an inexperienced professional'. Sometimes a patient tells us: 'could this tonsillitis be glandular fever? The doctor may accept the suggestion, or he may feel challenged, and answer: 'Of course it is not, what happens to you is very different from glandular fever'. Although the subsequent clinical evolution could be compatible with glandular fever, it will cost you lot consider this possibility. How different would have been to reply: 'I do not think so, but I will take your concern into account'! That would have allowed him to rectify things without the emotional cost, because most often 'anchoring' a diagnosis is, our self-esteem. To illustrate this relationship between anchoring the diagnosis and the clinician’s self-esteem, see figure 3.4 and imagine that in this example the patient says: 'No, doctor, you are wrong. Actually I had this cough before you put me on enalapril'. At this point the professional's self-esteem comes into play. If he reframes the interview and he denies his first hypothesis, the professional may be under the impression that his authority in front to the patient will be compromised. But this is just his impression. The truth is that patients appreciate it when clinicians rectify the diagnosis when there is a reason to do so. They like the clinician's certainty, but more so they appreciate flexibility.

 

But there is also a diagnostic anchoring or social origin. For example: we force the diagnosis of depression to explain the behaviour of a patient who has assaulted his wife, and thus give him a second chance within the family. In this case, the doctor may know that the diagnosis is not scientifically rigorous, but commitment and scientific certainty are confused. Other times, we insist on the nature of the disease of alcoholism, to anchor the patient's duties of their sick role: taking disulfiram, attending a therapeutic group, etc.

 

Consequently when we issue a diagnosis our prestige is anchored to this diagnosis. The clinician with years of experience uses expressions like 'it is possible that you have...' 'Everything seems to indicate...' Being careful in the expression used for a diagnosis gives us the freedom to modify it more easily in the future. And when the patient is pointing us towards a particular diagnostic hypothesis, it costs very little to say: 'I will take this into account'. However, other times it must be limited to anchoring the patient to their reality: 'we call your disease alcoholism'. (We will discuss the feature of anchoring the patient to their reality when we talk about motivational interviewing.)

 

Thought by criteria versus intuitive thought

We have discussed how to reach diagnoses and how at an emotional level our status is committed when we issue a diagnosis (anchored).

Let's move into the innermost mechanisms of this process. This should allow us to bring ourselves closer to strategies to prevent clinical errors. Let's see first of all, the role played by intuition (the 'clinical eye'), in the diagnostic process.

 

Maybe you have taken your car to a mechanic and they told you: 'it would be appropriate to change the rear tyres'. Or maybe you have been told: 'you must change the rear tyres, have look like here and here the tread has been lost'. Which one is more convincing? Something similar happens when we say to the patient: 'It would be a good idea for you to stop smoking', rather than saying, 'you must stop smoking because your cardiovascular risk is over 20%'. In the second case, the patient receiving the information realises that we are applying criteria to make the statement.

 

Thinking and making decisions based on criteria ('given the clinical circumstances it is appropriate for to me to do such a diagnostic or therapeutic action') leads to thinking in a more structured way, based on the experience accumulated from many people and, possibly, algorithms, interventions tested by clinical trials (evidence based medicine), or suggested by experts. This seems like a superior way to make clinical decisions rather than by just using clinical intuition. The expert is the precisely the person who has criteria for a very specific and simultaneously complex reality. But even the expert has several limitations:

 

  1. They have limitations in acquiring all the criteria that health sciences record for a specific clinical situation.
  2. Also even knowing all the information, only some of it comes to their mind. It is very different reflecting on a clinical case than being in front of a patient and having to make a decision in front of the patient.
  3. And, if that was not enough, reality is always full of subtleties and exceptions. The evidence which applies to a hypertensive diabetic may not be of value if they also have renal failure.

 

 Let's now analyse intuitive thinking. Instead of acting by criteria, this is a type of thought which operates with analogies: 'this situation is similar to such a situation, so I need to do...' It is therefore a style of thought characterised by:

 

  1. It is very fast. It establishes links with similar situations and proposes ways to resolve the situation by using past experiences as role model(s). This has the advantage of immediacy, but the disadvantage that it can easily deceive us (bias, memorable cases, recent experiences, etc.) (MR Esteve, 1996)
  2. Sometimes it acts as a red flag that says: Warning! Take care with this patient. This patient has something which goes beyond the apparent. We blur the reality with shades of doubt, surprise or perplexity, whereas thought based on criteria can only see routine, and black and white. It is a counterpoint to the analytical thinking and it enriches it.

 

That is to say, when intuitive thought prevails, it impoverishes us because we leave aside our enormous flow of formalized experience which is, ultimately, what provides the expert with security. But when using thought by criteria, if the interviewer is simultaneously able to listen at a second level to their intuitive thought, it reinforces and enriches their thought process. For example: 'this patient complaints of epigastric pain but his gastroscopy and ECG are normal; he is a very anxious patient with a lot of stress, but it conveys to me the experience of organic pain, I am worried, I am going to request an abdominal CT scan. And the CT scan shows an abdominal tumour. We must say at this point the doctor could have considered their decision driven by the desire to practice defensive medicine, when actually; the clinical reasoning has been more complex. It is so complex as to be unapparent and quick. The term defensive medicine should be saved for decisions made only to satisfy the patient's expectations (in this case should be better called satisfying medicine). But our doctor has been able to capture information about of an organic pain which the patient didn’t know how to express, and he has listened to a voice which alerted him to a serious cause for the pain, a voice which was not supported by any formal criteria.

 

However, if we restrict ourselves to only reflect on the successes of intuitive thought, we would have not do justice to reality. The truth is that on many occasions our intuition is more or less wrong: 'it is time to do a gastroscopy, but I am sure it will be negative, this patient exaggerates a lot! And in the end the exaggerating patient is found to have a stomach lymphoma. It is here where the clinician, knowing it or not, liking it or not, is always educating their intuition in a process we have named double subjective contrast.

 

How does educating our intuitive thought work? We convert the intuitions which experience has shown us are true, into criteria through double subjective contrast. This process is normally done in two separate visits which are chronologically spaced. Imagine we see a patient with jaundiced skin.

First subjective contrast: What is the intuitive assessment I do of this patient? Is the skin tone normal? The patient's conjunctivae are not jaundiced, there is no hepatomegaly... But I can't trust it. I am going to order some blood tests.

 

Second subjective contrast: The patient comes back with normal blood test results, so we think: 'I thought this patient was sicker that he really was, as I noted his skin was jaundiced and considered this a red flags sign. But this type of skin is normal in some people. In summary: 'from now on I will consider this skin type as a variant of normality'. By doing this, we have transformed an initial intuition into a criterion, but a criteria 'for me', as I don't have enough data to sustain it in the scientific community.

 

A creative professional practice has internal dialogues such as this one: 'although nobody has taught me this, the tremor I see in this patient must be due to anxiety, because it disappears when the patient takes anxiolytic medication; from now on, I will interpret it this way'. Double subjective contrast is the natural process of acquiring experience, because it is a thought process built up with 'for me' criteria. When the clinician is aware of this 'for me' process, they have a very rich source of different possible research projects: To convert the different criteria into validated criteria for other clinicians. Isn't that precisely what makes a person an expert?

 

A first approach to the decision rules (heuristics).

It is said that expert thought is thought regulated by criteria: 'in this situation, this is the corresponding action'. These criteria have been named action rules, decision rules, schemata or heuristics too. We will take these words as synonyms. There are a few ages of the clinician in relation to the acquisition and enrichment of the rules of action. Initially, at the stage of students, we assimilate these criteria as absolute truths. In a mature phase, the most creative of our professional practice, we are doubtful of these criteria, we are willing to change them when we open ourselves up to intuitive thought and, above all, we are open to recognize our point of perplexity ('I don't know this'). We apply this double subjective contrast to move from intuition to 'for me' criteria. In the decline of our career (not in a chronological sense, but when mediocrity, i.e. when we don't want to work better, takes its toll) we could become closed to absolute truths again, incapable of creatively rethinking the reality. We return to the seed, as Alejo Carpentier would say. So, the model we defend postulates that in the creative phase, we add more and more rules of action (or heuristics), of growing complexity to our way of acting. We improve our expertise because each clinical situation activates more complex rules of action, which take into account more possibilities, they offset more risks or leave more doors open to rectify adverse clinical courses. However, these rules of action are applied, without thought; they have been thought through before, when we proposed to implement them. In time we stop thinking about them and we simply incorporate them as habits or beliefs. The characteristic underlying habits is automated intelligence. The mechanical character of the habit it is the reason why it requires a great deal of effort to perform an analysis to detect which rules of action we are using and if they are incorrect, to proceed to change them. This process occurs in layers or strata, so heuristics or action rules which are obsolete remain in the deep memory, but they stay. We named them low level heuristics and they are normally decision rules which take a small effort to apply. On the other hand, high level heuristics are decision rules that force us to make a greater number of decisions to resolve the clinical situation (table 3.6).

 

 

Table 3.6.

High and low level heuristics.

Low level

  • Very little reflection or elaboration by the subject.

  • They are usually learned in early life or early in training.

  • They are comfortable to apply, with low energy consumption.

  • They are often the «minimum» that should be done in a particular situation.

High level

  • They appear after conscious reflection, i.e., doubting the first rules of decision (those rules of decision making which constitute 'tacit knowledge')

  • They respond to a more sophisticated analysis of the situation, and in general they foresee unfavourable courses which had not previously been considered.

  • Their application consumes more mental and physical energy.

 

 At this point, we would like draw the reader’s attention to a phenomenon we call a fall of heuristics. When fatigue, time pressure, a heavy workload or a flood of emotional feelings such as anger, shame or resentment assails us (table 3.7) we may experience what we call a 'heuristic fall', and we would want to solve a clinical situation by applying decision rules which require little energy investment. At this point, low-level heuristics show up. If we use a psychoanalytic term, we could say that we suffer a regression, since we have to use a rule of action that we learned in the initial stages of our training.

 

Table 3.7.

Factors limiting our performance

Lack or excess of emotional activation

  • Fatigue

  • Transient cognitive problems (for example; sleep disorders, alcohol consumption, etc.).

  • Lack of motivation

  • The professional rushes to finish the consultation and start another activity.

  • Feeling overwhelmed, feeling we can't cope, many sensory stimuli at the same time.

  • Being flooded by other extreme emotions, whether positive or negative.

Lack or excess of hedonic tone

  • Hostility of the patient.

  • Hostility of the professional them self.

  • Somatic discomfort or pain of the clinician.

  • The expectation that if a particular hypothesis is confirmed, it will create more work.

  • Allowing ourselves areas of magical thinking: for example 'I am not going to now think he may have cancer as he is such a nice person!’

 

 

If we use legal terms we would talk of negligent conduct. Imagine the following scenario:

 

A patient with colicky hypogastric pain, with a previous history of renal stones. High level heuristic: 'before diagnosing renal stones, we must examine the patient and perform a urine dipstick to check for blood in the urine'. Low level heuristic: 'there is sufficient evidence with a colicky type of pain and a previous history of kidney stones , so I will maintain this diagnosis even if the physical examination or urine dipstick are negative, so it is not worth complicating things by arranging further investigations'.

 

Observe in Figure 3.5 the heuristic fall. Basically, if we sometimes act negligently it is not because we want to harm the patient, but because we think that their problem can be solved without making a great deal of effort. We estimate the energy that we have to invest in the resolution of the case, and when it is scarce, we are satisfied by the use of low-level heuristics.

 

In Figure 3.5 illustrates what happens in the case of renal stones: we move from an error rate of 1/250 to an error rate of 1/100 when applying low level heuristics (these rates are purely indicative). That is what happens when restrictive factors act: for example if we are tired, nervous, irritable or overwhelmed by other patients who require our immediate attention, etc. In such circumstances, we simply assume more risks, 'we cut to the chase', and we close the interview more precipitously than is desirable, thanks to low level heuristics. But to let these low level rules of action operate, we need the presence of a cognitive alibi! A justification of the type: 'you've done what you had to do, you can close the interview', or 'at this time of the morning, I've done too much, I can close the interview'. However, a hidden dissatisfaction may remain, even when the restrictive factor disappears (e.g. tiredness). Maybe a few hours or days later we will say, (as if we have seen the scene rewound): 'I dealt with that patient badly!’ When a judge declares that we have acted negligently because we have not considered the possibility of an ovarian torsion, the only thing that we can claim is that we were under the influence of one or some of restrictive factors we list in table 3.7.

 

Fig-3.5

Figure 3.5.

Due to the presence of restrictive factors, the clinician resolves the clinical situation with a low level decision rule (heuristic). This means a reduction in their expertise.

 

 

 

These restrictive factors activate cognitive alibis which allow the use of low level heuristics. This is one of the most important points of the rational-emotive model.

 

Let's go back to the central theme with all of these concepts: what is the skill of the clinician? On one hand, to classify their knowledge and experience in order to know how to apply it to specific cases. In other words: create rules of action which are increasingly adapted to the complexity of real life. And on the other hand, to ask with a good cognitive activation ('energy'): 'am I doing what I should?', 'Am I acting under a cognitive alibi?’

Reader let us demonstrate another example. A registrar learns in A&E that: 'a red eye with sticky discharge and a gritty sensation in the eye equals conjunctivitis, and the patient must be given X'. Unfortunately this low-level heuristic ends by giving the clinician very serious problems, so he adopts another one:

 

'In face of a red eye, I will only say it is conjunctivitis if there is not much pain, there is only peripheral erythema and no erythema around the iris, and in case I have any doubt, I will exclude the presence of keratitis with a fluorescein test.'

 

He carried out the first part of the pathway (which by the way, is always at the mercy of the specific advances of each specialty). But now the second part remains: to apply the more complex heuristic at 4 o'clock in the morning during a particularly tough on call. What counts here will be: a) to detect cognitive alibis, and to say to himself: 'no, although I am tired the best option is to do things well, so I'm going to apply the rule of high level action'; b) to have good 'regulation of the optimal work area’, i.e. to arrive at 4 o'clock in the morning as clear as possible. Let's look at this last concept, which we have not yet discussed.

 

Optimal regulation of the working zone

It is not the same working with back pain as without it. It is not the same sleeping well as having sleep apnoea. From such obvious facts, Apter (1989) built up the inverted emotional theory, which in essence says: there is an ideal working zone which is attained by a good body tone and good cognitive activation. But be careful, because (as Figure 3.6 shows) either an 'excessively' good body tone or an excessive cognitive activation can damage our performance. Equally an excessively relaxed body tone is as much of an enemy as excessive activation, and boredom is as detrimental as excitation.

 

But, the reader may ask: are we aware of our body tone (or hedonic) or our cognitive activation? In general, no we're not. There is a circular nature of emotions by which every emotion justifies itself. This occurs too frequently when we are angry, we are too angry to see that our anger is not proportionate; when we are tired of driving, we tend to be too tired to consider that it would be wise to stop at a rest area, etc. We call this the circular nature of emotions. The person who comes out of this circle surrounding them has learned to regulate their work area by using indicators external to their self. For example: 'I'm more tired than I seem, because my handwriting is terrible. Or: 'I'm raising my voice too much... I am passionate and that can lead me to maintain my approach too rigidly'.

 

Fig-3.6

Figure 3.6.

Apter’s Theory of emotional investment. The golfer who repeatedly fails will move from excitation to anxiety. Another player who is excessively relaxed will get bored. The intensity of the emotions influences how we classify them.

 

 

The restrictive factors can displace us from our optimal working zone. Therefore we must equip ourselves with indicators so we ensure our maximum expertise.

 

Insurance expertise

Clinicians have developed different strategies to avoid making mistakes. We call all of this set of strategies secure expertise (as it refers to strategies to maintain expertise, rather than a global prevention of errors). Knowing and preserving our optimal working area is part of this effort. In this section we will address two more area: a) Interviewing skills, and b) signalling of clinical situations. The first is a global strategy, while the second refers to potentially dangerous situations or situations where we were previously wrong. Organisational and educational measures definitely have greater importance in the prevention of errors, as noted from the organisational and systemic approach (Khon LT, 2000). But here, we are interested in the other more personal approach, which is by no means contradictory with the systemic one.

 

a) Questioning skills and compensation of typical mistakes which are mediated by our character

 

Epstein has proposed that the clinician is something like a musical instrument that should be tuned to make good music (Epstein RM, 1999). This analogy seems appropriate and not exaggerated. In the same way that a music lover will distinguish the way of playing Alicia de la Rocha from Arthur Rubinstein, our character determines the style in which we solve problems. It also determines the type of mistakes we make.

 

The concept of latent error refers to a type of error which will eventually appear over time. For example, a small defect in the design of a car engine results in the majority of cars of that particular model developing the same type of problem over time. When we apply this simile to clinicians, we think about a lack of knowledge and skills. This is undoubtedly an important factor in errors, but character is much more important. In Table 3.8, we summarise some key concepts in relation to latent errors and character.

 

Table 3.8.

Character and latent errors

The absent minded will no longer perform tasks they planned to do at some point in the interview. Correction: When an action to perform comes to mind, write it down as a 'plan' the in patient's medical records.

 

The dogmatic will neither look for, nor hear data which is discordant with their first hypothesis (unable to disprove themselves nor allow others to contradict them).

 

Correction: demonstrate to them that they are wrong. The dogmatic person will never accept that they are wrong on their own, someone else needs to demonstrate or prove to them that they are wrong. The dogmatic will only leave their dream when it is perceived feel weak or fragile.

 

The impulsive may want to solve the discursive process based on only minimal data ('I have it'). Correction: here there is pathology, usually due to a lack of time in the consultation; we must learn to slow down and give ourselves time to prevent premature closure before completing the basic interview plan. Ask yourself regularly: would I be able to write a clinical report with the data I've just acquired?

 

The insecure will try to rule out the most remote possibilities, with an over use of investigations; other times they will take their lead from certain intuitions or expectations of the patient or other clinicians, without forming or taking into account their own intuitions and expectations. Ultimately their decisions are governed by avoiding tension and anxiety, and secondarily, to reach a diagnosis. Correction: learn to always form your own judgment, meaning that having a personal opinion does not need to be equivalent of others in order to deal with other people.

 

The perfectionist will consider all the details and possibilities over and over again, without a proper balance between time, effort and importance of the clinical situation. Going over the details will hinder them in seeing 'the most important' information from the patient.

 

Correction: ultimately the perfectionist tries to alleviate their anxiety of the unknown by exploring all possibilities; their efforts should be directed to follow protocols, without exaggerating them, in the conviction that they are sufficient consensus. Excess anxiety in the clinician can also result in iatrogenic problems, for example: by stimulating the patients' hypochondriasis.

 

 

Together with this knowledge of our characteristics, we must have a working habit which avoids making the mistakes that any clinician can routinely commit. Any professional with experience realises that they have a method of questioning themselves which oversees the most important processes. For example:

 

– Am I sufficiently activated/awake?

 

This habit of questioning oneself leads one to regulate the optimal working area, of which we have spoken above. Observe our handwriting, our fatigue when getting up from the chair, etc; they are good 'external' indicators.

 

– Am I opening my situations' library in the most extensive possible way?

 

This is closely related to the previous point. Sometimes we fail to recall a set of situations which are similar to the one we are trying to solve. This is due to the fact that we may be in a non-optimal working state; nervous, bewildered or simply tired. On such occasions we must stop, take a pen and paper and wonder: which similar situations have I been in previously? What is happening to this patient?

 

–Should the interview be reframed, thinking of new hypotheses?

 

First hypotheses arise effortlessly, but rethinking these first hypotheses requires a lot of energy, especially if we are thinking of closing the interview soon. This question should also arise when the patient comes to the clinic to see us repeatedly.

 

– Do I have enough data to write a medical report?

 

This habit of questioning prevents a premature closure of the exploratory phase. We have already mentioned the danger of finding data that corroborates our first hypotheses and not pursuing the basic interview plan.

 

– Am I closing the interview too quickly? And more specifically: Am I being victim of a cognitive alibi in order to prematurely close the interview?

 

This question should automatically jump out at us when we are not in tune with the patient's expectations, or when the hypothesis we propose does not clearly cover the whole symptomatic complex with which the patient presents, or when we suspect that we have 'falsely' closed the interview.

 

The clinician must choose from errors or faults detected in their clinical practice which of these questions (or other similar questions) is worth incorporating as a habit. This aspect is relevant: these are questions which should appear automatically during the interview, and they must be complied with. Only in this way can they act as insurance of our expertise.

 

b) Signalling of clinical situations

 

Clinicians put beacons with red lights on the clinical situations which can cause us to make errors. We have introduced the term of keys to force certain actions precisely to emphasise that the presence of a particular piece of information in the history or examination should always make us take a specific action. Another important concept is the red/green flag symptoms or signs, that is, data that without being as conclusive as above, it points to potentially serious or non- serious diseases. A headache which is worse in the morning, with sickness and vomiting would be a red flag, while a headache with extensive periods of remission between attacks, would point us towards a benign cause (green light). The clinician who organises things well in their mind puts the type of keys to force certain actions and the red/green flag symptoms and signs as indelible signals of forced processing, more tired they are. The analysis of situations that have led us to make mistakes is always of great interest. Sharing clinical experience with other professionals is also sharing our mistakes, and to facilitate things for our colleagues by showing them beacons with red lights from risky situations we have experienced. In this sense we can say that teamwork is a great opportunity to learn.

 

The depth of a diagnosis, the diagnostic statement and the biopsychosocial model

We have analysed the microscopic mechanisms of decision making, and it's time to recover a broader perspective. We have a patient with a health problem in front of us, and we ask ourselves: 'What should I do so that this patient is happy with my professional services?' This question marks to a great degree the initial framing of the interview (our intentionality). However, we cannot stay here. The professional who wants to make the most of the clinical act also wonders:

 

– Beyond patient's presenting complaint and expectations, what could the patient's problems and health needs be?

 

There is a real gulf between professionals who move within the framework of the demand (simple models of illness) and those who move within the framework of needs. Just as there are specific diagnostics ('bronchitis', 'pressure ulcers', etc.), there are also macro-diagnosis type: 'empty nest family', 'submissive caretaker’, 'domestic abuse', 'chronic social and family stress', etc. These macro-diagnoses unfortunately are not listed in the bibliography, despite the fact that they are the key to an extensive and at the same time profound biopsychosocial approach. When we apply them they give us diagnostic depth, and the physician reaches a depth of understanding equal to a nursing professional. By way of example:

 

  • The chronically ill patient's carer who falters (feeling overwhelmed by their carer duties), manifesting in depressive symptoms, arthralgia with or without myalgia, insomnia and often projects their unease about the patient, the object of their care; sometimes the carer behaves like a sick companion (meaning the companion who projects their symptoms on another relative who operates or represents the sick role).

  • An abused woman may present with chronic pain, usually denying emotional conflicts, although she shows non-verbal expressions of sadness, she has low self esteem and she sometimes makes requests to be referred to different specialists and earn the sick role, hoping to be better treated in their environment. As a result, another very complicated case.

  • The male patient with obesity and peripheral insulin resistance usually has other cardiovascular risk factors, such as: a sedentary lifestyle, high blood pressure, smoking, high CRP, dyslipidemia, etc. All of these factors should be considered to calculate his overall cardiovascular risk.

 

Macro-diagnoses combine biological and psychosocial aspects, so they have a versatility which is different from a classic diagnostic approach. Compare:

 

'Mr. Green you have a very strong cold and I recommend...' with:

'Mr Green you have a cold, but in addition, smoking and a lack of exercise are seriously harming your health. I also note anxiety in relation to your work.'

 

Immediate objection from the reader: how can we make such broad approaches with the few minutes we have? Answer: We cannot always nor should we always bring to the relationship everything we think of the patient, especially when we think that it will generate rejection. It is also true that this will take a few minutes, but less time than we usually believe it will. When the clinician takes a good focused history and defines the presenting complaint well, makes a map of complaints and they use the multiple complaints technique properly, many of these difficulties disappear as if by magic. For example:

 

'Mrs. Taylor, although we can only talk about your knee today, do not forget that you should start taking your antidepressant in anticipation to the start of autumn'.

'Mr. Duffield you have bronchitis. I will offer you some treatment, but I would like to show you your list of medical problems: I can see you have diabetes, high blood pressure and you smoke. If we calculated your cardiovascular risk it is 24%. We consider this percentage to be high. We should take action immediately. What do you think?'

 

An in depth approach is not part of the mere addition of diagnostics. This would be nonsense. The depth comes because clinician has models in their head, as we saw earlier. Imagine for a moment three patients with headache, the first one is a struggling carer, the second an abused woman, and the third a man with lots of risk factors... would they not have a very different approach? Reflection that leads to another approach: is this approach not precisely the biopsychosocial approach? A biopsychosocial focus, if we are allowed the license, with 'packaged' knowledge at the Schon way (1987), not so much as a 'new scientific paradigm', but a deeper and broader clinical practice than the classic 'one symptom = one treatment' (Borrell F, 2002). In Table 3.9 we summarise some of the features that permeate this type of practice.

 

Table 3.9

Biopsychosocial focus: proposals for practice

  1. The clinician considers their target to achieve an intrinsically therapeutic emotional component in the established relationship.

  2. The clinician legitimises any reason for consultation, not just the biomedical or psychosocial problems which are validated with a diagnosis (depression, bulimia, insomnia...).

  3. The clinician distinguishes between health demand and health need and apply macro-models where the psychosocial and biological aspects are considered.

  4. The clinician has an ethical duty to not lie about what they know of the patient, except for situations where not withholding this information would be more harmful. They also have the duty to place their own agenda in the care relationship and influence the patient's beliefs about the best therapeutic options, being aware not to cross explicit limits the patient has set. Sometimes this principle represents the courage of raising the psychosocial origin of certain somatisations, breaking cycles of chronic medicalisation or iatrogenic problems, but always avoiding causing a greater evil than the condition that we are trying to alleviate.

  5. The clinician facing a diagnosis with some complexity applies a methodology that initially avoids taking position towards either exclusively biological or exclusively psychosocial diagnostic hypotheses, i.e. avoids focusing on only one aspect of the case.

  6. The clinician will wherever possible, have the social and family perspective of their patients, and will appreciate the patients’ social support networks.

  7. The clinician will apply rational decision techniques based on evidence, but also being aware of the biases introduced by their own emotions. The dangers of making mistakes when seeing 'heart sink' patients are huge.

  8. The clinician must get used to periods of uncertainty, during which he must not give in to the temptation to give meaning to symptoms from non definitive data. There is no 'closed case'. Each patient is a reality in constant evolution. If we don't give the patient the opportunity to surprise us 'now', he will also surprise us tomorrow.

 

 

Summary

Packages of history taking skills that should be automated

  • First package: active listening skills: showing interest, facilitations, repeating sentences, empathy.

  • Second package: questions for a focused history: how is the pain? How long has it been going on? Chronological history of the pain, where? Any radiation, what triggers the pain and what makes it better, are there any related symptoms, risk factors.

  • Third package: questions for a psychological history: how do you sleep at night? What is your mood like? Are you very worried or do you have lots of things to think about? Are there any serious problems at home or at work?

  • Fourth package: questions for a patient-centred history: Do you have any idea about what this pain could be? How does this pain affect your life? How do you think I can help? Or what do you think should be done to improve this situation? Has something happened that has changed your life or had an impact on you? Do you have any problems at home or at work that could influence you?

 

Technical mistakes in the exploratory part

  • Intuitive interviewers and field dependence

  • Focalised interviewers

  • Blocking patients and interviewers who ask a lot but… by using closed questions

  • Skimming too quickly over the psychological aspects

 

Package of techniques to increase diagnostic capacity

Quality data from a concentrated informant who prepares their responses verbally and prevents automatic responses. An interviewer who is capable of reframing their first hypotheses, practicing the technique of reading textual data, inverse hypotheses, and clinical report technique, among other techniques.

 

Expertise and insurance expertise

Control over the optimal working zone, learning about our tolerance to critical stress, latent errors resulting from our character, questioning habits, signalling with red flags. Macro-diagnosis and biopsychosocial practice.

 

 

Bibliography

Apter MJ, Reversal Theory: Motivation, Emotion and Personality. Londres: Routledge, 1989. Aseguinolaza L, García-Campayo J, Tazón MP, Grupos Microbalint. En: Tazón MP, García-Campayo J, Aseguinolaza L, eds. Enfermería. Relación y Comunicación. Madrid: DAE, 2000.

Barragán N, Violan C, Martín Cantera C, Ferrer-Vidal D, González-Algas J. Diseño de un método para la evaluación de la competencia clínica en atención primaria. Aten Primaria. 2000 Nov 30;26(9): 590-594.

Borrell F, Sunyol R, Porta M, Holgado M, Bosch JM, Jiménez J. Capacidad para detectar y registrar problemas de salud, en relación a la calidad de la entrevista clínica. I WONCA European Regional Conference on Family Medicine. Barcelona, 1990.

Borrell F. Exploración Física Orientada a los Problemas Aten Primaria 2002; 30(1): 32-45.

Borrell F. CD ROM: Exploración Física Orientada a los Problemas-CD-ROM I. FMC Noviembre 2002. Boucher FG, Palmer WH, Page G. The evaluation of clinical competence. Can Fam Physician 1980;26: 151-152.

Burack RC, Carperter RR. The predictive value of the presenting complaint. J Fam Pract 1983; 16(4):749-754.

Engel G. The need for a new medical model: a challenge for biomedicine. Science 1977; 196 (4286): 129-136.

Engel G. The clinical application of the biopsychosocial model. Am J Psychiatry 1980; 137(5):535-544.

Epstein RM. Mindful Practice. JAMA 1999; 282(9): 833-839.

Esposito V, Schorow M, Siegel F. A problem oriented precepting method. J Fam Pract 1983;17(3):469-473.

Esteve MR. Teoría de la decisión (II): el teorema de Bayes. En: Godoy A. Toma de decisiones y jui- cio clínico. Madrid: Pirámide, 1996; págs. 215-221.

Gask L, Goldberg D, Lesser AL, Millar T. Improving the psychiatric skills of the general practice trai- nee: an evaluation of a group training course. Med Educ 1988; 22: 132-138.

Gross DA. Patient satisfaction with time spent with their physician. J Fam Pract 1998; 47(2): 133. Khon LT, Corrigan JM, Donaldson S. To Err is Human. Building a Safer Health System. Institute of Medicine. Washington DC: National Academy Press, 2000.

Kraytmann M. El diagnóstico a través de la historia clínica. Madrid: IDEPSA, 1983.

Prados JA, Muñoz E, Santiago JM, Cebrià J, Bosch JM, Peguero E, Borrell F. Evaluación de la compe- tencia en Habilidades de exploración clínica de los Médicos de Familia en situación real: Capacidad de detección de signos en pacientes con hallazgos clínicos confirmados previamente. Influencia en el manejo Diagnóstico-terapéutico. FISS 2003.

Ratey, JJ. El Cerebro: manual de instrucciones. Barcelona: Mondadori, 2002; pág. 270.

Shea SC. La entrevista psiquiátrica. El arte de comprender. Madrid: Harcourt Saunders Elsevier Sciences, 2002.

Suñol R. Correlación entre los procesos y los resultados de la Entrevista Clínica: su aplicación a los programas de calidad en Atención Primaria. Tesis Doctoral, Universidad Autónoma de Barcelona. Barcelona, 1992.

 

 

Back to the index

Previous chapter